PERSPECTIVE
Physicians and Implicit Bias: How Doctors May Unwittingly
Perpetuate Health Care Disparities
Elizabeth N. Chapman, MD1,5, Anna Kaatz, MA, MPH, PhD4
, and Molly Carnes, MD, MS1,2,3,4,5
1
Department of Medicine, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI, USA; 2
Department of Psychiatry, University of Wisconsin-Madison,
Madison, USA; 3
Industrial & Systems Engineering, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, USA; 4
Center for Women’s Health Research,
University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, USA; 5 William S. Middleton Memorial Veterans Hospital Geriatric Research Education and
Clinical Center, Madison, WI, USA.
Although the medical profession strives for equal treatment
of all patients, disparities in health care are prevalent.
Cultural stereotypes may not be consciously endorsed, but
their mere existence influences how information about an
individual is processed and leads to unintended biases in
decision-making, so called “implicit biasâ€. All of society is
susceptible to these biases, including physicians. Research
suggests that implicit bias may contribute to health care
disparities by shaping physician behavior and producing
differences in medical treatment along the lines of race,
ethnicity, gender or other characteristics. We review the
origins of implicit bias, cite research documenting the
existence of implicit bias among physicians, and describe
studies that demonstrate implicit bias in clinical decisionmaking. We then present the bias-reducing strategies of
consciously taking patients’ perspectives and intentionally
focusing on individual patients’ information apart from
their social group. We conclude that the contribution of
implicit bias to health care disparities could decrease if all
physicians acknowledged their susceptibility to it, and
deliberately practiced perspective-taking and individuation
when providing patient care. We further conclude that
increasing the number of African American/Black physicians could reduce the impact of implicit bias on health
care disparities because they exhibit significantly less
implicit race bias.
KEY WORDS: implicit bias; health care disparities; physicians.
J Gen Intern Med 28(11):1504–10
DOI: 10.1007/s11606-013-2441-1
© Society of General Internal Medicine 2013
INTRODUCTION
At graduation, most newly minted physicians take the
Hippocratic Oath, vowing to treat all patients as respected
individuals. When they enter practice, physicians are charged
to apply principles of evidence-based medicine and meet
performance measures intended to produce uniformly highquality medical care. Despite these efforts, disparities in health
care persist. The Institute of Medicine notes that these
differences result from multiple factors, but “bias, stereotyping, [and] prejudice … on the part of the health care
providers†play a role.1 The mere existence of cultural
stereotypes about social groups (e.g. women, men, Blacks,
Whites) can influence one’s behavior toward and judgment of
individuals from that stereotyped group.2 As opposed to
explicit prejudices (e.g., believing women are not as competent surgeons as men or that men are unemotional), implicit
bias occurs without conscious awareness and is frequently at
odds with one’s personal beliefs.3 Because populations in the
U.S. that experience the greatest health disparities also suffer
from negative cultural stereotypes, implicit bias among
physicians may impact clinical decision-making in ways that
perpetuate health care disparities.4,5
ORIGINS OF IMPLICIT BIAS
Humans base their perceptions of reality on received
information and experiences reinforced until they become
automatic. This ability makes human decision-making
efficient and likely provided an evolutionary advantage.
Stereotypes are well-learned sets of associations between
some trait and a social group. A stereotype may be accurate
at a group level (e.g., Wisconsinites root for the Green Bay
Packers), but inaccurate at an individual level (i.e., some
root for the Chicago Bears). Experiments first published by
Devine demonstrated two forms of bias in a race context
that result from stereotypes.2 In this now classic work,
Devine found that White participants explicitly endorsing
prejudiced beliefs toward Blacks as well as those who
holding more egalitarian beliefs were equally able to
produce lists of cultural stereotypes about Blacks (e.g.,
rhythmic, uneducated, hostile). She also found that following exposure to stereotype-activating words, highprejudiced and low-prejudiced participants equally interpreted ambiguous behavior of a Black character in a
vignette as hostile. However, when asked to write their
thoughts about Black Americans, low-prejudiced participants produced more thoughts with themes of equality,
JGIM
Received December 14, 2012
Revised February 15, 2013
Accepted March 18, 2013
Published online April 11, 2013
1504
(e.g., “it is unfair to judge people by their colorâ€), while
high-prejudiced participants expressed more stereotypecongruent thoughts. Together, these experiments showed
that common cultural experiences create awareness of
stereotypes, which can be automatically activated in ways
that bypass deliberate thought and influence one’s judgment
in unintended and unacknowledged ways. Devine described
these as automatic (implicit) and controlled (explicit)
aspects of prejudice.2
Since Devine’s early work, the existence of implicit bias
from the automatic activation of race, gender, ethnic, age
and other stereotypes has been demonstrated to influence
judgment of and behavior toward individuals from stereotyped groups. Implicit bias is also called “unconscious†or
“non-conscious†bias6,7 and, as noted by Devine and others,
often differs starkly from explicit beliefs.3 Multiple studies
with randomized, controlled designs confirm that simply
knowing about a stereotype distorts processing of information about individuals. Resulting outcomes include unintentionally rating identically performing Black students as
less academically capable than Whites,8,9 and evaluating
identically credentialed female applicants as less qualified
than males.10,11
Implicit bias develops early in life from repeated
reinforcement of social stereotypes. Implicit pro-White bias
occurs among children as young as 3 years old throughout
the world.12–15 In a study of children and adults, Baron et
al. found that explicit beliefs about race became more
egalitarian with age, but implicit race bias remained
unchanged.15 Implicit and explicit beliefs about other
characteristics, like age and gender, may follow similar
patterns. Despite evolution of a person’s explicit beliefs,
enduring implicit bias appears to have significant influence
on behavioral interactions with individuals from stereotyped
groups. A representative study by Dovidio et al. showed
that college students’ implicit race bias had no relationship
to self-reported egalitarian attitudes, yet predicted friendliness in interactions with a Black student.16
PHYSICIANS AND IMPLICIT BIAS
Physicians are not immune to implicit bias. Indeed,
uncertainty and time pressure surrounding the diagnostic
process may promote reliance on stereotypes for efficient
decision-making.17–20 Physician training emphasizes group
level information, like population risk factors, and may
expose trainees to minorities in unfavorable circumstances
of illness or addiction, reinforcing stereotypes. Finally,
physicians’ vast knowledge of scientific data may create a
strong belief in their personal objectivity, promoting bias in
decision-making.21
The most commonly used measure of implicit bias is the
Implicit Association Test (IAT), a computerized timed dual
categorization task that measures implicit preferences by
bypassing conscious processing.22 The Black/White:
Good/Bad IAT is most frequently used to assess implicit
race bias. Its participants press different computer keys
to sort photographs of African American and Caucasian
American faces as either “Black†or “White†and then
sort words like “joy, wonderful, glorious†and “agony,
horrible, evil†into “Good†and “Bad†categories,
respectively. Next, participants repeat the task, sequentially being asked to press one key when shown a
“Black†stimulus or a “Bad†word and a different key
when shown a “White†stimulus or a “Good†word, and
vice versa. Participants must answer as quickly as
possible, increasing reliance on automatic responses.
Shorter response times for White-Good and Black-Bad
associations compared with White-Bad and Black-Good
reflect a pro-White implicit bias. The magnitude of the
difference in milliseconds correlates with the degree of
one’s implicit bias.22 In the many studies that use this
version of the IAT, the vast majority of test-takers show
pro-White bias.23–25
The IAT has been used to measure implicit bias in
physicians (Table 1). Green et al. assessed explicit and
implicit race bias among internal medicine and emergency
medicine residents7 and found significant pro-White bias
despite no explicitly reported preference for Whites over
Blacks. Participants also implicitly associated Blacks with
uncooperativeness, particularly regarding procedures.7 Although Sabin et al. found less implicit race bias among
pediatricians compared with other physicians, implicit (but
not explicit) bias was still present, linking compliance with
being White.26 Sabin et al. also examined implicit race bias
in physicians as a whole using data from Harvard’s Project
Implicit© website, which allows volunteers to take various
versions of the IAT online.27 Overall, the 2,535 website
participants who reported having an MD degree showed
significant pro-White bias.28 Others have confirmed that
even in the absence of explicit race bias, implicit preference
for Whites among physicians is common.29,30 The degree of
implicit race bias varies by physician race and gender. In
Sabin et al.’s data, the presence of pro-White bias was
significant among physicians of all racial groups except
African Americans, who were neutral, while women
showed less implicit race bias than men.28 Less—but not
zero—pro-White bias has also been found among nonWhite vs. White resident physicians and medical
students.7,31,32
Race is not the only area where physicians demonstrate implicit bias. Schwartz et al. recruited physicians
and researchers attending a conference on obesity. The
participants completed four separate IATs associating
images of obese and non-obese people with being good
or bad, motivated or lazy, smart or stupid, and valuable
or worthless. Regardless of their explicit beliefs,
JGIM Chapman et al.: Physicians and Implicit Bias 1505
participants implicitly associated obese people with
negative cultural stereotypes.33 Studies examining clinical
decision-making suggest that implicit bias in other areas,
including gender and age, may also be present.34–39
Table 1. Research Relevant to Implicit Bias and Clinical Decision-Making
Lead Author,
Year
Type of
Bias
Measured
Stereotyped
Group
Results
Studies showing implicit
bias in physicians via the
Implicit Association
Test (IAT)
Schwartz, 2003 Explicit and
implicit
Obese
individuals
Implicit negative obese bias present and did not
correlate with explicit obese bias.
Sabin, 2008 Explicit and
implicit
Black
individuals
Despite a lack of explicit race bias, pro-White implicit
bias present, but did not correlate with responses to
vignette.
Sabin, 2009 Implicit Black
individuals
Significant pro-White bias present, regardless of race,
but Black physicians had a less than non-Black
physicians, and female physicians had less than male
physicians.
White-Means,
2009
Explicit and
Implicit
Black
individuals
Medical students had significant pro-White implicit
bias, throughout training. Black and biracial students
tended to have less than White, Hispanic and Asian
students.
Studies linking IATmeasured bias with clinical
decision-making or patient
perceptions
Cooper, 2012 Implicit Black
individuals
Implicit pro-White bias and an association of Whites
with compliance found. Increased provider implicit
bias associated with physician verbal dominance and
less positive perceptions of interactions by Black
patients.
Green, 2007 Explicit and
implicit
Black
individuals
No explicit race bias but significant pro-White implicit
bias found. More pro-White bias associated with lower
referral rates of Black vignette patient for
thrombolysis. Participants aware of study intent more
likely to treat patients similarly, regardless of their
implicit race bias.
Penner, 2010 Explicit and
implicit
Black
individuals
Physicians with low explicit but high implicit race bias
were rated more poorly by Black patients and had
lower rates of patient satisfaction than those with low
explicit and implicit bias. Physicians with both high
explicit and high implicit biases were not rated as
poorly as those with low explicit and high implicit biases.
Sabin, 2012 Explicit and
implicit
Black
individuals
Pro-White bias present; participants implicitly
associated Black patients with non-adherence despite
absent explicit biases. Implicit pro-White bias
associated with providing opioids less for Black
children with postoperative pain.
Vignette studies suggesting
the presence of implicit
bias
Borkhoff, 2008 None Women Despite being otherwise identical, male patients referred
for TKA significantly more often than females, even
when adjusting for covariates.
Chapman, 2001 None Women Physicians are more likely to diagnose male patients
with COPD when provided with history and
examination alone. Providing PFTs consistent with
COPD mitigated this difference.
Madan, 2001
and 2006
None Elderly
individuals
Breast conservation therapy recommended significantly
more frequently for younger vignette patients, even
after adjusting for covariates.
Reuben, 1995 Explicit Elderly
individuals
Varying degrees of explicit bias about elderly patients
present. Elderly patients were treated significantly less
aggressively than young patients
Uncapher, 2000 None Elderly
individuals
Depression and suicidality recognized in both elderly and
younger patients; treatment offered to elderly less and
providers less optimistic regarding treatment benefit.
Real clinical scenarios
where bias is suggested
Drwekci, 2011 None Black
individuals
Nurses asked to take the patient’s perspective offered
equal pain treatment, regardless of patient race,
whereas those not asked to do so recommended more
pain medication for White patients.
Todd, 1993 None Hispanic
individuals
Hispanic patients significantly more likely not to receive
analgesia in the ER compared to non-Hispanic
patients, even when adjusting for confounders
Todd, 1994 None Hispanic
individuals
Hispanic patients and non-Hispanic patients did not
differ significantly in pain ratings, and there was no
difference in physician–patient disparity in pain ratings
for Hispanic patients vs. non-Hispanic patients
Todd, 2000 None Black
individuals
Patients had similar pain ratings regardless of race, yet
Black patients were significantly less likely to receive
pain medication, even after adjusting for covariates
COPD chronic obstructive pulmonary disease; IAT implicit association test; PFT pulmonary function test; TKA total knee arthroplasty
1506 Chapman et al.: Physicians and Implicit Bias JGIM
THE EFFECTS OF IMPLICIT BIAS ON MEDICAL
DECISION-MAKING
Demonstrating that physicians have measurable implicit bias
does not prove that this bias affects patient–doctor interactions
or alters the treatment patients receive. However, research
supports a relationship between patient care and physician bias
in ways that could perpetuate health care disparities.4 In a
qualitative study of patient–doctor communication, Cooper et
al. found that physicians’ implicit pro-White bias on the IAT
correlated with Black patients’ perceptions of poorer communication and lower quality care.30 Penner et al. found that
Black patients were less satisfied with physicians who had low
explicit but high implicit race bias, rating them as less warm,
friendly, and team-oriented compared to physicians with equal
degrees of implicit and explicit bias.32 The small number of
family medicine residents in this study showed less implicit
bias than physicians in other studies, possibly because most of
them were born outside the U.S. and were caring only for nonWhite patients. Dovidio et al. also found that the perception of
an interaction between White physicians and Black patients
was affected by a physician’s implicit race bias, even in the
absence of explicit biases.16 Such negative perceptions could
alter their behavior in ways that reduce adherence, return for
follow-up, or trust and thus contribute to disparities in care.
Perceptions of poorer care or communication alone may
not directly alter the quality of treatment provided, but
research also supports a link between disparate treatment
decisions and implicit provider bias. This research exists in
two forms: studies comparing treatment recommendations
for patients who are identical except for social category
information, and studies directly measuring implicit bias
and then determining the correlation between measured bias
and physicians’ decisions. The former studies assume
providers are not intentionally acting with any explicit bias,
while the latter group quantifies explicit and implicit bias.
Two studies found that Black patients seen in emergency
departments receive less analgesia than White patients.40,41
Hispanic patients in one study were seven times less likely
to receive opioids in the emergency room than nonHispanic patients with similar injuries, even when adjusting
for confounders.42 These findings were duplicated in Black
patients.43 In a follow-up study, researchers assessed
physicians’ ability to quantifying pain in Hispanic patients
compared to non-Hispanic patients. They found that
physicians could accurately judge patients’ pain severity
regardless of ethnicity yet still provided less analgesia to
Hispanic patients with severe injuries.44 Even in the
absence of direct measurement of implicit bias, these
compelling data suggest that physicians make treatment
decisions that divide patients with similar clinical presentations along lines of race or ethnicity.
Sabin et al. measured implicit race bias in pediatricians
who were then provided with identical vignettes randomly
identifying a patient as either Black or White. Providers
who had a high degree of implicit pro-White bias were
significantly more likely to disagree with the standard of
care—providing opioid analgesics to the patient—than those
with a low degree of bias, while explicit race attitudes had no
bearing on treatment.29 Green et al. demonstrated that
providers’ implicit attitudes about race may contribute to the
disparity in care of patients with acute coronary syndrome,7
where Black patients are less likely than Whites to receive
appropriate therapies.45–49 Internal medicine and emergency
medicine residents took the Black/White:Good/Bad IAT and
also answered questions about a vignette of a patient with
chest pain randomly designated as Black or White. Residents
with greater implicit bias were significantly less likely to
recommend thrombolysis for Black patients than those with
less bias. Again, implicit bias measured in this study was
associated with different treatment of theoretical patients that
corresponds with a real-life health care disparity.
Implicit gender bias among physicians may also unknowingly sway treatment decisions. Women are three times less
likely than men to receive knee arthroplasty when clinically
appropriate. Although physicians denied that patient gender
influenced decisions to refer patients for the procedure,50–52 a
study by Borkhoff et al. challenged this. In the study, orthopedic
surgeons and family practitioners received vignettes featuring a
patient with moderate unilateral knee pain and a radiograph
revealing osteoarthritis. Identical vignettes were randomly
ascribed to a female or male patient. The statistically significant
odds ratio for referring the male vs. female patient for
arthroplasty was 22.1 for orthopedic surgeons and 2.21 for
family practitioners.38 Implicit assumptions based on stereotypes that men are more stoic than women or more likely to
engage in rigorous activities that would benefit from joint
replacement may contribute to the disparity and adversely
influence the care of individual female patients.38,51,52
Implicit stereotype-based bias may also contribute to gender
differences in the diagnosis of chronic obstructive pulmonary
disease (COPD), even in the face of near comparable smoking
rates between men and women and women’s increased
susceptibility to the disease. Chapman et al.39 created a clinical
vignette of a middle-aged patient presenting with a chronic
cough and a smoking history. All vignettes were identical
except for the randomly assigned patient gender. Female
patients were more likely to receive a diagnosis of asthma or
a non-respiratory problem, while identical male patients were
more likely to be diagnosed with COPD.39 Implicit bias was not
measured, but assumptions that women are less likely to smoke
and more apt to manifest anxiety as respiratory complaints may
have played a role in this diagnostic disparity.
REDUCING THE IMPACT OF IMPLICIT BIAS
With growing evidence that implicit bias in physician
decision-making makes a significant contribution to perpetJGIM Chapman et al.: Physicians and Implicit Bias 1507
uating health care disparities, it is critical to find ways to
reduce its impact. Conceptualizing implicit bias as a “habit
of mind†provides a useful framework for developing
interventions.53–55 This approach allows mobilization of a
large body of work on facilitating intentional behavioral
change.53 As with any behavioral change, individuals need
to become aware of their habitual engagement in an
undesirable behavior and be provided with strategies to
increase self-efficacy to engage in a new desirable behavior.56 The study by Green et al. provides an example of how
simply increasing physicians’ awareness of their susceptibility to implicit bias changes behavior.7 A subset of
participants who were aware that bias was a focus of the
investigation were significantly more likely to recommend
thrombolysis for Black patients, even if they had a high
degree of implicit pro-White bias.7
Although awareness is important, as with clinical efforts
to change patients’ undesirable health behaviors,57 it is not
sufficient to reduce the automatic, habitual activation of
stereotypes and the subsequent impact of implicit bias in
medical decision-making. One strategy that appears effective in reducing implicit bias is individuating. Individuating
involves conscious effort to focus on specific information
about an individual, making it more salient in decisionmaking than that person’s social category information (e.g.
race or gender). Lebrecht et al. tasked White participants to
indicate whether they had previously seen a Black face
presented to them. Those trained to differentiate Black faces
were more accurate than untrained participants, and their
accuracy correlated with reduced implicit race bias.58
Chapman et al.’s study of gender disparities in COPD
diagnoses supports the benefit of individuation in reducing
the influence of implicit bias in medical practice. In the
study, the initial gender differences in diagnosis were
eliminated when physicians were provided with spirometry
data consistent with COPD.39 Specific, individuated patient
information prevents physicians from filling in partial
information with stereotype-based assumptions. Unfortunately, medicine is often practiced under time-pressured
circumstances with incomplete information.
Another strategy to mitigate the impact of implicit bias is
perspective-taking. Originally described by Galinsky et
al.,59 this is a conscious attempt to envision another
person’s viewpoint and can reduce implicit bias in social
interactions.59,60 Drwecki et al. applied perspective-taking
to a clinical setting. In this study, nurses were shown
pictures of either White or Black patients with genuine
expressions of pain and asked how much pain medication
they recommended. Nurses told to use their best judgment
recommended significantly more pain medication for White
than Black patients, whereas nurses instructed to imagine
how the patient felt recommended equal analgesic treatment
regardless of race.61 It may thus be possible to reduce
disparities in pain treatment if providers actively attempt to
take patients’ perspectives. In addition to behavioral strategies
aimed at individual physicians, the contribution of implicit
race bias to health care disparities could be reduced on a
population level by increasing the number of African
American/Black physicians because they consistently demonstrate less race bias.7,28,31 Certainly society is not divided
along lines of race, gender, age or weight alone, and the impact
of implicit social bias is likely far more complex than available
data can capture. Nonetheless, individuating and perspectivetaking are bias-mitigating strategies that should be effective
regardless of a patient’s social group.
CONCLUSION
Despite the best intentions of physicians to provide equal
treatment to all, disparities linger and may lead to
unacceptable increases in morbidity and mortality for some.
Many factors have helped create these disparities, including
implicit bias, an unintentional, unacknowledged preference
for one group over another. Implicit bias is present in
physicians and correlates with unequal treatment of
patients. We suggest the contribution of implicit bias to
health care disparities could be reduced if all physicians
acknowledged their susceptibility to such bias and deliberately practiced perspective-taking and individuation. Additionally, increasing the number of African American/Black
physicians could reduce the impact of implicit bias on some
health care disparities because they exhibit significantly less
implicit race bias. Although challenging, these strategies
may help create a practice of medicine that embodies the
ideals and guiding principles that attract physicians to the
field.
Acknowledgements: Dr. Chapman is a Geriatric Fellow at the
Madison GRECC and Dr. Kaatz is a postdoctoral fellow at the Center
for Women’s Health Research. This is GRECC Manuscript No. 2013-
14. Dr. Carnes’s research on implicit bias is supported by NIH grant
no. R01 GM088477 and DP4 GM096822.
Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare that they do not have a
conflict of interest.
Corresponding Author: Elizabeth N. Chapman, MD; William S.
Middleton Memorial Veterans Hospital Geriatric Research Education
and Clinical Center, Madison VA GRECC, 2500 Overlook Terrace,
Madison, WI 53705, USA (e-mail: [email protected]).
REFERENCES
1. Nelson A. Unequal treatment: confronting racial and ethnic disparities
in health care. J Natl Med Assoc. 2002;94(8):666–8.
2. Devine PG. Stereotypes and prejudice: their automatic and controlled
components. J Personal Soc Psychol. 1989;56(1):5–18.
3. Devine PG, Plant EA. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. San
Diego: Academic; 2012.
1508 Chapman et al.: Physicians and Implicit Bias JGIM
4. Institute of Medicine. Unequal Treatment: Confronting Racial and
Ethnic Disparities in Health Care. Smedley BD, Stith AY, Nelson
AR, eds. Washington, D.C. : The National Academies Press 2001
5. Dovidio JF, Penner LA, Albrecht TL, Norton WE, Gaertner SL,
Shelton JN. Disparities and distrust: the implications of psychological
processes for understanding racial disparities in health and health care.
Soc Sci Med. 2008;67(3):478–86.
6. Association of American Medical Colleges. What You Don’t Know: The
Science of Unconscious Bias and What to Do About It in the Search and
Recruitment Process. Available at: https://www.aamc.org/initiatives/
leadership/recruitment/178420/unconscious_bias.html. Accessed
February 14, 2013.
7. Green AR, Carney DR, Pallin DJ, Ngo LH, Raymond KL, Iezzoni LI, et
al. Implicit bias among physicians and its prediction of thrombolysis
decisions for black and white patients. J Gen Intern Med.
2007;22(9):1231–8.
8. Biernat M, Manis M. Shiftings standards and stereotype-based judgments. J Personal Soc Psychol. 1994;60:485–99.
9. Hodson G, Dovidio JF, Gaertner SL. Processes in racial discrimination:
differential weighting of conflicting information. Pers Soc Psychol Bull.
2002;28(4):460–71.
10. Isaac C, Lee B, Carnes M. Interventions that affect gender bias in hiring:
a systematic review. Acad Med. 2009;84(10):1440–6.
11. Moss-Racusin CA, Dovidio JF, Brescoll VL, Graham MJ, Handelsman
J. Science faculty’s subtle gender biases favor male students. Proc Natl
Acad Sci U S A. 2012;109(41):16474–9.
12. Bigler RS, Liben LS. A developmental intergroup theory of social
stereotypes and prejudice. Adv Child Dev Behav. 2006;34:39–89.
13. Newheiser AK, Olson KR. White and Black American Children’s Implicit
Intergroup Bias. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2012;48(1):264–70.
14. Dunham Y, Baron AS, Banaji MR. From American city to Japanese
village: a cross-cultural investigation of implicit race attitudes. Child
Dev. 2006;77(5):1268–81.
15. Baron AS, Banaji MR. The development of implicit attitudes. Evidence of
race evaluations from ages 6 and 10 and adulthood. Psychol Sci.
2006;17(1):53–8.
16. Dovidio JF, Kawakami K, Gaertner SL. Implicit and explicit prejudice
and interracial interaction. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2002;82(1):62–8.
17. Tversky A, Kahneman D. Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and
Biases. Science. 1974;185(4157):1124–31.
18. Tait RC, Chibnall JT, Kalauokalani D. Provider judgments of patients
in pain: seeking symptom certainty. Pain Med. 2009;10(1):11–34.
19. Croskerry P. Achieving quality in clinical decision making: cognitive
strategies and detection of bias. Acad Emerg Med. 2002;9(11):1184–
204.
20. Martell RF. Sex bias at work: the effects of attentional and memory
demands on performance ratings of men and women. J Appl Soc
Psychol. 1991;21(23):1939–60.
21. Uhlmann EL, Cohen GL. I think it, therefore it‘s true’: Effects of selfperceived objectivity on hiring discrimination. Organ Behav Hum Decis
Process. 2007;104(2):207–23.
22. Greenwald AG, McGhee DE, Schwartz JL. Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. J Pers Soc
Psychol. 1998;74(6):1464–80.
23. Nosek BA, Banaji M, Greenwald AG. Harvesting implicit group attitudes
and beliefs from a demonstration web site. Group Dyn: Theory Res Pract.
2002;6(1):101–15.
24. Greenwald AG, Poehlman TA, Uhlmann EL, Banaji MR. Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: III. Meta-analysis of
predictive validity. J Personal Soc Psychol. 2009;97(1):17–41.
25. Eisenberg T, Johnson SL. Implicit racial attitudes of death penalty
lawyers. Cornell Law Facutly Publications, Paper 353. 2004. Available at:
http://scholarship.law.cornell.edu/facpub/353. Accessed on February
14, 2013.
26. Sabin JA, Rivara FP, Greenwald AG. Physician implicit attitudes and
stereotypes about race and quality of medical care. Med Care.
2008;46(7):678–85.
27. Corporation I. Project Implicit.
28. Sabin J, Nosek BA, Greenwald A, Rivara FP. Physicians’ implicit and
explicit attitudes about race by MD race, ethnicity, and gender. J Health
Care Poor Underserved. 2009;20(3):896–913.
29. Sabin JA, Greenwald AG. The influence of implicit bias on treatment
recommendations for 4 common pediatric conditions: pain, urinary tract
infection, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, and asthma. Am J
Public Health. 2012;102(5):988–95.
30. Cooper LA, Roter DL, Carson KA, Beach MC, Sabin JA, Greenwald
AG, et al. The associations of clinicians’ implicit attitudes about race
with medical visit communication and patient ratings of interpersonal
care. Am J Public Health. 2012;102(5):979–87.
31. White-Means S, Zhiyong D, Hufstader M, Brown LT. Cultural competency, race, and skin tone bias among pharmacy, nursing, and medical
students: implications for addressing health disparities. Med Care Res
Rev. 2009;66(4):436–55.
32. Penner LA, Dovidio JF, West TV, Gaertner SL, Albrecht TL, Dailey
RK, et al. Aversive racism and medical interactions with Black patients:
a field study. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2010;46(2):436–40.
33. Schwartz MB, Chambliss HO, Brownell KD, Blair SN, Billington C.
Weight bias among health professionals specializing in obesity. Obes Res.
2003;11(9):1033–9.
34. Uncapher H, Arean PA. Physicians are less willing to treat suicidal
ideation in older patients. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2000;48(2):188–92.
35. Reuben DB, Fullerton JT, Tschann JM, Croughan-Minihane M.
Attitudes of beginning medical students toward older persons: a fivecampus study. The University of California Academic Geriatric Resource
Program Student Survey Research Group. J Am Geriatr Soc.
1995;43(12):1430–6.
36. Madan AK, Aliabadi-Wahle S, Beech DJ. Age bias: a cause of
underutilization of breast conservation treatment. J Cancer Educ.
2001;16(1):29–32.
37. Madan AK, Cooper L, Gratzer A, Beech DJ. Ageism in breast cancer
surgical options by medical students. Tenn Med. 2006;99(5):37–8. 41.
38. Borkhoff CM, Hawker GA, Kreder HJ, Glazier RH, Mahomed NN,
Wright JG. The effect of patients‘ sex on physicians’ recommendations
for total knee arthroplasty. CMAJ. 2008;178(6):681–7.
39. Chapman KR, Tashkin DP, Pye DJ. Gender bias in the diagnosis of
COPD. Chest. 2001;119(6):1691–5.
40. Heins JK, Heins A, Grammas M, Costello M, Huang K, Mishra S.
Disparities in analgesia and opioid prescribing practices for patients with
musculoskeletal pain in the emergency department. J Emerg Nurs.
2006;32(3):219–24.
41. Miner J, Biros MH, Trainor A, Hubbard D, Beltram M. Patient and
physician perceptions as risk factors for oligoanalgesia: a prospective
observational study of the relief of pain in the emergency department.
Acad Emerg Med. 2006;13(2):140–6.
42. Todd KH, Samaroo N, Hoffman JR. Ethnicity as a risk factor for
inadequate emergency department analgesia. JAMA.
1993;269(12):1537–9.
43. Todd KH, Deaton C, D’Adamo AP, Goe L. Ethnicity and analgesic
practice. Ann Emerg Med. 2000;35(1):11–6.
44. Todd KH, Lee T, Hoffman JR. The effect of ethnicity on physician
estimates of pain severity in patients with isolated extremity trauma.
JAMA. 1994;271(12):925–8.
45. Petersen LA, Wright SM, Peterson ED, Daley J. Impact of race on
cardiac care and outcomes in veterans with acute myocardial infarction.
Med Care. 2002;40(1 Suppl):I86–96.
46. Allison JJ, Kiefe CI, Centor RM, Box JB, Farmer RM. Racial differences in the medical treatment of elderly Medicare patients with acute
myocardial infarction. J Gen Intern Med. 1996;11(12):736–43.
47. Canto JG, Allison JJ, Kiefe CI, Fincher C, Farmer R, Sekar P, et al.
Relation of race and sex to the use of reperfusion therapy in Medicare
beneficiaries with acute myocardial infarction. N Engl J Med.
2000;342(15):1094–100.
48. Weitzman S, Cooper L, Chambless L, Rosamond W, Clegg L, Marcucci
G, et al. Gender, racial, and geographic differences in the performance of
cardiac diagnostic and therapeutic procedures for hospitalized acute
myocardial infarction in four states. Am J Cardiol. 1997;79(6):722–6.
49. Taylor HA Jr, Canto JG, Sanderson B, Rogers WJ, Hilbe J. Management and outcomes for black patients with acute myocardial infarction
in the reperfusion era. National Registry of Myocardial Infarction 2
Investigators. Am J Cardiol. 1998;82(9):1019–23.
50. Hawker GA, Wright JG, Coyte PC, Williams JI, Harvey B, Glazier R,
et al. Differences between men and women in the rate of use of hip and
knee arthroplasty. N Engl J Med. 2000;342(14):1016–22.
51. Coyte PC, Hawker G, Croxford R, Attard C, Wright JG. Variation in
rheumatologists‘ and family physicians’ perceptions of the indications for
and outcomes of knee replacement surgery. J Rheumatol.
1996;23(4):730–8.
52. Wright JG, Coyte P, Hawker G, Bombardier C, Cooke D, Heck D, et al.
Variation in orthopedic surgeons’ perceptions of the indications for and
outcomes of knee replacement. CMAJ. 1995;152(5):687–97.
JGIM Chapman et al.: Physicians and Implicit Bias 1509
53. Carnes M, Devine PG, Isaac C, Baier Manwell L, Ford CE, ByarsWinston A, Fine E, Sheridan J. Promoting institutional change through
bias literacy. J Divers High Educ. 2012;5(2):63–77.
54. Devine PG, Plant EA, Buswell BN, Oskamp S. Breaking the prejudice
habit: Progress and obstacles. Reducing Prejudice and Discrimination.
Mahwah, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers; 2000:185–
208.
55. Devine PG, Forscher PS, Austin AJ, Cox WTL. Long-term reduction in
implicit race prejudice: a prejudice habit-breaking intervention. J Exp
Soc Psychol. 2012;48:1267–78.
56. Bandura A, Kazdin, A.E. Social-cognitive theory. Encyclopedia of
psychology, Vol. 7. Washington, DC, New York, NY, USUS: American
Psychological Association, Oxford University Press; 2000:329-32.
57. Prochaska JO, DiClemente CC. Stages of change in the modification of
problem behaviors. Prog Behav Modif. 1992;28:183–218.
58. Lebrecht S, Pierce LJ, Tarr MJ, Tanaka JW. Perceptual other-race
training reduces implicit racial bias. PLoS One. 2009;4(1):e4215.
59. Galinsky AD, Moskowitz GB. Perspective-taking: decreasing stereotype
expression, stereotype accessibility, and in-group favoritism. J Pers Soc
Psychol. 2000;78(4):708–24.
60. Todd AR, Bodenhausen GV, Richeson JA, Galinsky AD. Perspective
taking combats automatic expressions of racial bias. J Pers Soc Psychol.
2011;100(6):1027–42.
61. Drwecki BB, Moore CF, Ward SE, Prkachin KM. Reducing racial
disparities in pain treatment: the role of empathy and perspectivetaking. Pain. 2011;152(5):1001–6.
1510 Chapman et al.: Physicians and Implicit Bias JGIM
Any academic writer who wishes to join our team of professional writers must possess all the following qualities:
To write an exemplary academic paper, you must have good critical thinking skills, possess the proper knowledge of the discipline, and be knowledgeable about applying an academic writing style.
As such we have a rigorous recruitment process. We only collaborate with professional academic writers. We believe in offering the highest quality academic writing services. Our writers pass various grammar and academic writing tests. They have to provide documents about their personal information and credentials to prove their level of expertise.
As a result, our clients receive papers that are thoroughly-researched, properly cited, and written within academic standards. We are proud that any academic writer from our writer's team can complete the paper at a high standard.
We work with the student’s budget because we know that students are usually on a budget majority of the time. We do not compromise on quality because of low prices. On the contrary, we love to foster a good relationship with our clients. That is why we charge our clients reasonable prices, and we are willing to negotiate and work with their budget.
It may be quite disturbing to decide whether to hire an academic writing company. We care about our client’s privacy and confidentiality. We never disclose your information to third parties. We never publish your paper online. You can use our academic writing service without any fear or anxiety.
Many students struggle with writing academic papers. Some barely have time to do their assignments because of their job and family responsibilities. Others have difficulty applying critical thinking skills or meeting time or assignment requirements. Whatever the reason is, you can always have time to do the things you love and other important things. All you need is a reliable and quality academic writing service. Unfortunately, even if you strongly desire to write the paper yourself, you sometimes face unexpected challenges. As we all know, life is unpredictable! Your teacher may be unconcerned about helping students and may not answer your questions. The good news is that there is a way out! You can hire an online academic writer to help you with your assignments. All you need to do is stipulate your paper requirements in the order form, and you can spend your time as you like.
Our company commits towards delivering high-quality custom papers to our clients. We seek to offer reliable essay writing services to our customers in various subject areas. Our customers are very valuable to us. As such, we commit to ensuring that they derive the utmost satisfaction from the essays we deliver. We have a mission to promote our clients' educational and professional lives by providing high-quality essays for their use. We also have a mission to offer a convenient essay writing system where our customers can easily order and pay for the services. We value quality and professionalism in our company.
We write papers on any subject area, and we also write various types of papers for various purposes. We have a team of able writers who are eager to help our customers with writing services of exceptional quality. We offer custom writing services for customers across the globe and charge affordable prices for our services. We are the best essay writing company offering personalized services to all our customers. We ensure our customers receive maximum satisfaction from the essays we deliver. Our website is the place to be if you are seeking high-quality essays.
By using our academic writing service, we match your paper subject with a writer with a degree in the subject. The writer is able to apply their skills, knowledge, and expertise to the paper. You receive an original, unique, well-research paper, properly cited and formatted. As such, you are assured of a high-quality paper.
Truth be told, writing assignments can be stressful and difficult for any student. There is no shame in getting academic writing help. When you search the term “get academic writing help” there are numerous sites that pop up on the results and our website is among them. So, why is it a great idea to choose us?
During your course, your instructor will assign various types of homework. Our academic writers can prepare essays, presentations, speeches, case studies, research papers, dissertations, thesis papers, and more. Our writer’s department is capable of tackling any assignment of any complexity easily. All you need is to give us detailed instructions to help our experts understand the task.After doing so, you can rest assured that everything is in control, and we will deliver a paper of unmatchable quality.
Are you a college or university student pursuing your Bachelor’s , Masters, PhD, or Doctorate degree? Sometimes juggling schoolwork with work, family and hobbies can seem like a daunting task. You have to sacrifice one or the other. The sole purpose of our website is to alleviate your academic burdens. We ensure that you do not fail in your classes and you get good grades consistently. We understand that there is a need for academic help.
We acknowledge that our clients are not dumb or lazy but only need academic life need help in order to live a balanced life and make ends meet. We make it our core priority to ensure that all assignments are done and submitted before the stipulated deadlines. All our writers are graduates. They are competent in handling the clients’ assignments. We step in to help you with any and all of your assignments. Our assignment help service ensures that you never miss a grade or deadline.
Students barely have time to read. We got you! Have your literature essay or book review written without having the hassle of reading the book. You can get your literature paper custom-written for you by our literature specialists.
Do you struggle with finance? No need to torture yourself if finance is not your cup of tea. You can order your finance paper from our academic writing service and get 100% original work from competent finance experts.
Computer science is a tough subject. Fortunately, our computer science experts are up to the match. No need to stress and have sleepless nights. Our academic writers will tackle all your computer science assignments and deliver them on time. Let us handle all your python, java, ruby, JavaScript, php , C+ assignments!
While psychology may be an interesting subject, you may lack sufficient time to handle your assignments. Don’t despair; by using our academic writing service, you can be assured of perfect grades. Moreover, your grades will be consistent.
Engineering is quite a demanding subject. Students face a lot of pressure and barely have enough time to do what they love to do. Our academic writing service got you covered! Our engineering specialists follow the paper instructions and ensure timely delivery of the paper.
In the nursing course, you may have difficulties with literature reviews, annotated bibliographies, critical essays, and other assignments. Our nursing assignment writers will offer you professional nursing paper help at low prices.
Truth be told, sociology papers can be quite exhausting. Our academic writing service relieves you of fatigue, pressure, and stress. You can relax and have peace of mind as our academic writers handle your sociology assignment.
We take pride in having some of the best business writers in the industry. Our business writers have a lot of experience in the field. They are reliable, and you can be assured of a high-grade paper. They are able to handle business papers of any subject, length, deadline, and difficulty!
We boast of having some of the most experienced statistics experts in the industry. Our statistics experts have diverse skills, expertise, and knowledge to handle any kind of assignment. They have access to all kinds of software to get your assignment done.
Writing a law essay may prove to be an insurmountable obstacle especially when you need to know the peculiarities of the legislative framework. Take advantage of our top-notch law specialists and get superb grades and 100% satisfaction.
$10 page
$13 page
$14 page
Our prices depend on the urgency of your assignment, your academic level, the course subject, and the length of the assignment. Basically, more complex assignments will cost more than simpler ones. The level of expertise is also a major determinant of the price of your assignment.
If you need professional help with completing any kind of homework, is the right place to get it. Whether you are looking for essay, coursework, research, or term paper help, or with any other assignments, it is no problem for us. At our cheap essay writing service, you can be sure to get credible academic aid for a reasonable price, as the name of our website suggests. For years, we have been providing online custom writing assistance to students from countries all over the world, including the US, the UK, Australia, Canada, Italy, New Zealand, China, and Japan.
Our cheap essay writing service has already gained a positive reputation in this business field. Understandably so, since all custom papers produced by our academic writers are individually crafted from scratch and written according to all your instructions and requirements. We offer APA, MLA, or a Chicago style paper in almost 70 disciplines. Here, you can get quality custom essays, as well as a dissertation, a research paper, or term papers for sale. Any paper will be written on time for a cheap price.
Using our cheap essay writing help is beneficial not only because of its easy access and low cost, but because of how helpful it can be to your studies. Buy custom written papers online from our academic company and we won't disappoint you with our high quality of university, college, and high school papers. Although our writing service is one of the cheapest you can find, we have been in the business long enough to learn how to maintain a balance between quality, wages, and profit. Whenever you need help with your assignment, we will be happy to assist you.
It might seem impossible to you that all custom-written essays, research papers, speeches, book reviews, and other custom task completed by our writers are both of high quality and cheap. It is surprising, but we do have some tricks to lower prices without hindering quality.
To start using our services, it’s enough to place a request like “I need a writer to do my assignment” or “Please, write an essay for me.” We have a convenient order form, which you can complete within minutes and pay for the order via a secure payment system. The support team will view it after the order form and payment is complete and then they will find an academic writer who matches your order description perfectly. Once you submit your instructions, while your order is in progress and even after its completion, our support team will monitor it to provide you with timely assistance.
Hiring good writers is one of the key points in providing high-quality services. That’s why we have entry tests for all applicants who want to work for us. We try to make sure all writers working for us are professionals, so when you purchase custom-written papers, they are of high quality and non-plagiarized.
Our cheap essay writing service employs only writers who have outstanding writing skills. The quality of all custom papers written by our team is important to us; that is why we are so attentive to the application process and employ only those writers who can produce great essays and other kinds of written assignments. All our writers are graduates. They are competent in handling the clients’ assignments. We step in to help you with any and all of your assignments. Our assignment help service ensures that you never miss a grade or deadline.
All our cheap essays are customized to meet your requirements and written from scratch. Our writers have a lot of experience with academic papers and know how to write them without plagiarism. Moreover, at our academic service, we have our own plagiarism-detection software which is designed to find similarities between completed papers and online sources. You can be sure that our custom-written papers are original and properly cited.
Our essay writing service has a 0% plagiarism tolerance. We are well aware of the dangers of plagiarism. Plagiarism is academic suicide. Our essay writing service ensures that all papers are original. We do not sell pre-written papers. All papers are written from scratch as per the instructions. We pass our papers through powerful anti-plagiarism software such as SafeAssign and TurnItIn.
Our cheap essay writing service tries to always be at its best performance level, so each customer who pays money for paper writing can be sure that he or she will get what is wanted. On the off chance that you don’t like your order, you can request a refund and we will return the money according to our money-back guarantee.
There can be a number of reasons why you might not like your order. If we honestly don’t meet your expectations, we will issue a refund. You can also request a free revision, if there are only slight inconsistencies in your order. Your writer will make the necessary amendments free of charge. You can find out more information by visiting our revision policy and money-back guarantee pages, or by contacting our support team via online chat or phone.
We know how important any deadline is to you; that’s why everyone in our company has their tasks and perform them promptly to provide you with the required assistance on time. We even have an urgent delivery option for short essays, term papers, or research papers needed within 8 to 24 hours.
We appreciate that you have chosen our cheap essay service, and will provide you with high-quality and low-cost custom essays, research papers, term papers, speeches, book reports, and other academic assignments for sale. We beat all deadlines. We can also handle urgent orders with deadlines as short as 1 hour. Our urgent paper writing service does not compromise on quality due to the short deadline. On the contrary, our essay writers have a lot of experience which comes in handy in such situations.
We provide affordable writing services for students around the world. That’s why we work without a break to help you at any time, wherever you are located. Contact us for cheap writing assistance. Our impeccable customer support team will answer all your questions and help you out with any issues.
Proceed to order pageDelivering a high-quality product at a reasonable price is not enough anymore.
That’s why we have developed 5 beneficial guarantees that will make your experience with our service enjoyable, easy, and safe.
You have to be 100% sure of the quality of your product to give a money-back guarantee. This describes us perfectly. Make sure that this guarantee is totally transparent.
Read moreEach paper is composed from scratch, according to your instructions. It is then checked by our plagiarism-detection software. There is no gap where plagiarism could squeeze in.
Read moreThanks to our free revisions, there is no way for you to be unsatisfied. We will work on your paper until you are completely happy with the result.
Read moreYour email is safe, as we store it according to international data protection rules. Your bank details are secure, as we use only reliable payment systems.
Read moreBy sending us your money, you buy the service we provide. Check out our terms and conditions if you prefer business talks to be laid out in official language.
Read more